How a child's language development can be helped by hand movements: Gesticulating makes words 'easier to understand'
comments
Spontaneously moving your hands to illustrate a point can help boost a child's language development, researchers say.
Gesture-plus-word combinations can make it easier for children to grasp what certain words and phrases mean - even during sign language.
The findings back up previous research which found gesticulating helps youngsters develop their language, learning and cognitive skills.
Researchers at the University of Chicago say gestures help children learn (stock image shown). Study says it helps kids develop their language, learning and cognitive skills. Gesture is a flexible way of communicating, one that can work with language to communicate or, if necessary, can itself become language
The new study, by Dr Susan Goldin-Meadow, from the University of Chicago, examined how gesturing contributes to language learning in hearing and in deaf children.
She concluded that gesture is a flexible way of communicating, one that can work with language to communicate or, if necessary, can itself become language.
'Children who can hear use gesture along with speech to communicate as they acquire spoken language, ' Dr Goldin-Meadow said.
'Those gesture-plus-word combinations precede and predict the acquisition of word combinations that convey the same notions.
'The findings make it clear that children have an understanding of these notions before they are able to express them in speech.'
In addition to children who learned spoken languages, Dr Goldin-Meadow studied children who learned sign language from their parents.
She found that they too use gestures as they use American Sign Language. These gestures predict learning, just like the gestures that accompany speech.
Finally, Dr Goldin-Meadow looked at deaf children whose hearing losses prevented them from learning spoken language, and whose hearing parents had not presented them with conventional sign language.
These children use homemade gesture systems, called homesign, to communicate.
Homesign shares properties in common with natural languages but is not a full-blown language, perhaps because the children lack 'a community of communication partners,' Dr Goldin-Meadow writes.
Nevertheless, homesign can be the 'first step toward an established sign language.'
In Nicaragua, individual gesture systems blossomed into a more complex, shared system when homesigners were brought together for the first time.
The gesture learning technique is apparently true for both hearing children and those who sign (research image shown). Gesture-plus-word combinations can make it easier for children to understand what certain words and phrases mean, which makes it easier for them to pick up language more quickly at school
These findings provide insight into gesture's contribution to learning.
Gesture plays a role in learning for signers even though sign itself is based around gestures.
As a result, gesture cannot aid learners simply by providing a second language. Rather, gesture adds imagery to the categorical distinctions that form the core of both spoken and sign languages.
Dr Goldin-Meadow concludes that gesture can be the basis for a self-made language, assuming linguistic forms and functions when other vehicles are not available.
But when a conventional spoken or sign language is present, gesture works along with language, helping to promote learning.
The study will be published in Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society .
Put the internet to work for you.
0 comments:
Post a Comment